Dyadic Adjustment Scale

Dyadic Adjustment Scale
Dyadic Adjustment Scale

DYADIC ADJUSTMENT SCALE (DAS)

Reference:

Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38, 15–28. Description of Measure:

A 32-item measure of relationship quality. The scale is divided into 4 subscales:

(1) Dyadic Consensus – degree to which respondent agrees with partner

(2) Dyadic Satisfaction -- degree to which respondent feels satisfied with partner

(3) Dyadic Cohesion –degree to which respondent and partner participate in activities together

(4) Affectional Expression –degree to which respondent agrees with partner regarding emotional affection.

Items have varying response scales (see scale below).

Abstracts of Selected Related Articles:

Graham, J. M., Liu, Y. J., & Jeziorski, J. L. (2006). The Dyadic Adjustment Scale: A reliability generalization meta-analysis. Journal of Marriage and Family, 68, 701-

717.

We conducted a reliability generalization meta-analysis to examine the internal

consistency of Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS; Spanier, 1976) scores across 91

published studies with 128 samples and 25,035 participants. The DAS was found to produce total and Dyadic cohesion, Consensus, and Satisfaction scores of acceptable internal consistency, although lower than those originally reported by Spanier

(1976). Reliability estimates of these scores did not differ by the sexual orientation, gender, marital status, or ethnicity of the sample. The Affective Expression subscale was found to produce scores with poor Cronbach's alpha across studies. Reliability

estimates of Affective Expression scores were highly influenced by sample

characteristics. The implications of these results are discussed as they relate to the use of the DAS in research.

Carey, M. P., Spector, I. P., Lantinga, L. J., & Krauss, D. J. (1993). Reliability of the Dyadic Adjustment Scale. Psychological Assessment, 5, 238-240.

This study examined the reliability of Spanier's (1976) Dyadic Adjustment Scale

(DAS). Middleaged men and women (/V = 158) completed the DAS on 2 occasions

separated by approximately 2 weeks. Separate alpha and stability coefficients were calculated for each of the 4 DAS subscales as well as the Total score. Coefficients

alpha ranged from .70 (for the 4-item Affectional Expression subscale) to .95 (for the 32-item Total score), Mdn = .87. Stability coefficients ranged from .75 (Affectional

Expression) to .87 (Total), Mdn = .81. Partial correlations revealed that the stability of the DAS was not influenced by subjects' age, educational attainment, number of children, relationship duration, or the length of the test-retest interval. These

results suggest the DAS and its 4 subscales are internally consistent and stable over the interval examined in this study.

Spanier, G. B. & Thompson, L. (1982). A confirmatory factor analysis of the Dyadic Adjustment Scale. Journal of Marriage and Family, 44, 731-738.

Evaluated the dyadic adjustment scale by reconsidering the factor structure of the

scale and its subscales using a maximum likelihood, confirmatory factor-analysis

procedure. Studied a new sample from the same geographical area. High reliability was confirmed for the overall scale.

Scale: Contact Multi Health Systems 1-800-456-3003 for permission to use items.

REVISED DYADIC ADJUSTMENT SCALE (RDAS) Reference:

Busby, D. M., Christensen, C., Crane, D. R., & Larson, J. H. (1995). A revision of the Dyadic Adjustment Scale for use with distressed and nondistressed couples: Construct

hierarchy and multidimensional scales. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 21, 289-308.

Description of Measure:

A 14-item scale designed to measure relationship satisfaction. The RDAS is a revised version of the original Dyadic Adjustment Scale (Spanier, 1976 –see this website for information on the original). The revised version offers improved psychometric properties, is shorter, and includes only 3 of the original 4 subscales:

(1) Dyadic Consensus – degree to which respondent agrees with partner

(2) Dyadic Satisfaction -- degree to which respondent feels satisfied with partner

(3) Dyadic Cohesion –degree to which respondent and partner participate in activities together

The items have varying response scales (see scale below).

Abstracts of Selected Related Articles:

Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38, 15–28.

This study reports on the development of the Dyadic Adjustment Scale, a new

measure for assessing the quality of marriage and other similar dyads. This factor

analytic study suggests four empirically verified components of dyadic adjustment to

be used as subscales (dyadic satisfaction, dyadic cohesion, dyadic consensus and

affectional expression).

Treboux, D., Crowell, J. A., & Waters, E. (2004). When “new” meets “old”: Configurations of adult attachment representations and their implications for marital functioning. Developmental Psychology, 40?295-314.

Two studies addressed the implications of concordance versus discrepancy of

attachment representations in individuals at 2 stages in their marital relationships.

Engaged (n _ 157) and dating (n _ 101) couples participated in a multimethod 6-year longitudinal study of adult attachment. Individuals completed the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI), the Current Relationship Interview (CRI), and various

questionnaires and were observed in interactions with partners. On the basis of AAI and CRI classifications, participants were placed in one of four groups:

SecureAAI/SecureCRI, SecureAAI/InsecureCRI, InsecureAAI/SecureCRI, or

InsecureAAI/InsecureCRI. Each of the configurations showed a particular pattern of behavior, feelings about relationships and the self, and likelihood of relationship

breakup. The findings of the studies address important points about the protective

effects of attachment security and have interesting implications for the extension of attachment theory into adulthood.

Graham, J. M., Liu, Y. J., & Jeziorski, J. L. (2006). The Dyadic Adjustment Scale: A reliability generalization meta-analysis. Journal of Marriage and Family, 68, 701-

717.

We conducted a reliability generalization meta-analysis to examine the internal

consistency of Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS; Spanier, 1976) scores across 91

published studies with 128 samples and 25,035 participants. The DAS was found to produce total and Dyadic cohesion, Consensus, and Satisfaction scores of acceptable internal consistency, although lower than those originally reported by Spanier

(1976). Reliability estimates of these scores did not differ by the sexual orientation, gender, marital status, or ethnicity of the sample. The Affective Expression subscale was found to produce scores with poor Cronbach's alpha across studies. Reliability

estimates of Affective Expression scores were highly influenced by sample

characteristics. The implications of these results are discussed as they relate to the use of the DAS in research.

Scale: Contact Multi Health Systems 1-800-456-3003 for permission to use items.

INVESTMENT MODEL SCALE (IMS) Reference:

Rusbult, C. E., Martz, J. M., and Agnew, C. R. (1998). The Investment Model Scale: Measuring commitment level, satisfaction level, quality of alternatives, and

investment size. Personal Relationships, 5, 357-391.

Description of Measure:

A 29-item scale that measures four constructs of the investment model (Rusbult, 1980 see below for abstract):

(1) Commitment level – degree one intends to persist in the relationship

(2) Relationship satisfaction – degree that the relationship fulfilled needs for intimacy, sex, companionship, security and emotional involvement.

(3) Quality of alternatives – degree that one believes the satisfaction needs (above) could be fulfilled in another relationship.

(4) Investment size -- measures perceptions of time invested, interconnected identity, memories, and shared experiences.

The Relationship Satisfaction, Quality of Alternatives, and Investment Size components each have Facet Items that are initially asked, prior to more global items. These facet items are concrete exemplars of each construct and are designed to prepare the respondent for the global items and are not measured in final analyses.

Respondents answer each item on a 9-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 8 (completely).

Here is a diagram of the Investment Model (from Rusbult et al., 1998):

Abstracts of Selected Related Articles:

Rusbult, C. E. (1980a). Commitment and satisfaction in romantic associations: A test of the investment model. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,16, 172-186

According to the investment model, attraction to and satisfaction with a relationship is a function of a comparison of the relationship outcome value to the individual's

expectations, or comparison level. Commitment to a relationship is said to be a

function not only of the relationship outcome value, but also the quality of the best available alternative and the magnitude of the individual's investment in the

relationship. The investment of resources serves to increase commitment by

increasing the costs of leaving the relationship. In Exp I, with 171 undergraduates, commitment to relationships increased with investment size and decreased with the value of alternatives, but was not appreciably affected by relationship costs.

Satisfaction/attraction significantly increased as relationship costs decreased. In

Exp II, with 111 undergraduates, satisfaction/attraction was predicted by

relationship reward value and relationship cost value. Commitment to relationships increased as relationship reward value and investment size increased and as

alternative value and relationship cost value decreased, although the effects of cost value were weak.

Rusbult, C. E. (1983). A longitudinal test of the investment model: The development (and deterioration) of satisfaction and commitment in heterosexual involvements.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 101-117.

Used a longitudinal study of heterosexual dating relationships to test investment

model predictions regarding the process by which satisfaction and commitment

develop (or deteriorate) over time. Initially, 17 male and 17 female undergraduates, each of whom was involved in a heterosexual relationship of 0-8 wks duration,

participated. Four Ss dropped out, and 10 Ss' relationships ended. Questionnaires

were completed by Ss every 17 days. Increases over time in rewards led to

corresponding increases in satisfaction, whereas variations in costs did not

significantly affect satisfaction. Commitment increased because of increases in

satisfaction, declines in the quality of available alternatives, and increases in

investment size. Greater rewards also promoted increases in commitment to

maintain relationships, whereas changes in costs generally had no impact on

commitment. For stayers, rewards increased, costs rose slightly, satisfaction grew, alternative quality declined, investment size increased, and commitment grew; for leavers the reverse occurred. Ss whose partners ended their relationships evidenced entrapment: They showed relatively low increases in satisfaction, but their

alternatives declined in quality and they continued to invest heavily in their

relationships.

Rusbult, C. E., Verette, J., Whitney, G. A,, Slovik. L. F., & Lipkus, I. (1991).

Accommodation processes in close relationships: Theory and preliminary empirical evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 53-78.

A theory of accommodation processes is advanced, and the results of 6 studies are

reported. Accommodation refers to the willingness, when a partner has engaged in a potentially destructive act, to inhibit impulses to react destructively and instead

react constructively. Studies 1 and 2 demonstrated that accommodation is lower

under conditions of reduced social concern and lower interdependence. Studies 3, 4, and 5 revealed that accommodation is associated with greater satisfaction,

commitment, investment size, centrality of relationship, psychological femininity,

and partner perspective taking and with poorer quality alternatives. Commitment plays a fairly strong role in mediating willingness to accommodate. Study 6 showed that couple functioning is associated with greater joint and mutual tendencies to

inhibit destructive reactions. Study 6 also demonstrated that self-reports of

accommodation are related to relevant behavioral measures.

Scale: Contact author for permission to use items.

PERSONAL ASSESSMENT OF INTAMACY IN

RELATIONSHIPS SCALE

Reference:

Schaefer, M. T. & Olson, D. H. (1981) Assessing intimacy: The PAIR Inventory, Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 1, 47-60.

Description of Measure:

A 36-item measure of relationship intimacy, encompassing five different factors and one “faking” scale. The five factors are:

(1) Emotional Intimacy – feeling closeness, ability to share feelings, and be supported without defensiveness.

(2) Social Intimacy – having common friends and social network.

(3) Sexual Intimacy – sharing affection, touching, physical and sexual closeness.

(4) Intellectual Intimacy – sharing ideas and experiences about life and work.

(5) Recreational Intimacy – sharing of experiences, common pastimes and involvement in activities.

The scale can either be phrased in terms of how the relationship “is now” or it can be phrased in terms of how the relationship “should be” (or both), depending on what the researcher wishes to study.

Respondents answer each item on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).

Abstracts of Selected Related Articles:

Moore, K. A., McCabe, M. P., & Stockdale, J. E. (1998). Factor analysis of the Personal Assessment of Intimacy in Relationships Scale (PAIR): Engagement communication and shared friendships. Sexual and Relational Therapy, 13, 361-368.

The Personal Assessment of Intimacy in Relationships (PAIR) was developed by

Schaefer & Olson (1981) to assess both the actual and ideal levels of intimacy in

relationships. Attempts to replicate the original factor structure have not been

reported. In Stage 1 of the present study, 157 volunteers (34 males, 123 females;

mean age 27.76 years) who were currently in committed relationships completed the PAIR. Factor analysis failed to replicate the original structure but yielded a reliable, independent three-factor solution: engagement, communication and shared

friendships. There were no gender differences on these factors. Confirmatory factor analysis failed to confirm this three-factor solution in a group of 145 clients (77

males, 68 females; mean age 35.79 years) presenting to a clinic for the treatment of sexual dysfunction. Principal components analysis yielded a unifactorial solution.

These results suggest that people from the general population demonstrate three

independent but increasingly involved aspects of intimacy, ranging from

engagement to communication to shared friendships. People with sexual dysfunction seem to experience a decrement in all aspects of intimacy. The implications of these findings for the treatment of sexual dysfunction are discussed.

Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., Impett, E. A., & Asher, E. R. (2004). What do you do when things go right? The intrapersonal and interpersonal benefits of sharing positive events.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 228-245.

Four studies examined the intrapersonal and interpersonal consequences of seeking out others when good things happen (i.e., capitalization). Two studies showed that

communicating personal positive events with others was associated with increased daily positive affect and well-being, above and beyond the impact of the positive

event itself and other daily events. Moreover, when others were perceived to respond actively and constructively (and not passively or destructively) to capitalization

attempts, the benefits were further enhanced. Two studies found that close

relationships in which one’s partner typically responds to capitalization attempts

enthusiastically were associated with higher relationship well-being (e.g., intimacy, daily marital satisfaction). The results are discussed in terms of the theoretical and empirical importance of understanding how people “cope” with positive events,

cultivate positive emotions, and enhance social bonds.

Reis, H. T. & Franks, P. (2005). The role of intimacy and social support in health outcomes: Two processes or one? Personal Relationships, 1, 185-197.

That intimacy and social support are related to an individual's health and well-being has often been noted. The present study had two goals. First, we intended to

establish whether intimacy and social support were related to mental and physical health in a large, representative community sample. Second, we sought to determine whether intimacy and social support make unique contributions to predicting

health, as a step toward developing a model of the relation between these processes.

Results strongly supported the initial hypothesis that intimacy and social support

were both related to health status. We also found that the effects of intimacy on

well-being were mediated by social support, but that the effects of social support

were not mediated by intimacy. We therefore concluded that the health-promoting

benefits of intimacy most likely occur because intimate relationships are likely to

engender higher levels of social support. Distinguishing unique and shared

prediction effects is a generic concern for disciplines that study variables that are

naturally correlated in real life, such as in the field of personal relationships. Scale: Contact Dr. Olson for permission to use items.

RELATIONSHIP ASSESSMENT SCALE Reference:

Hendrick, S. S. (1988). A generic measure of relationship satisfaction. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 50, 93–98.

Description of Measure:

A 7-item scale designed to measure general relationship satisfaction. Respondents answer each item using a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (low satisfaction) to 5 (high satisfaction). Abstracts of Selected Related Articles:

Aron, A., Norman, C. C., Aron, E. N., McKenna, C., & Heyman, R. E.(2000). Couples’ shared participation in novel and arousing activities and experienced relationship quality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 273-284.

Using a newspaper questionnaire, a door-to-door survey, and 3 laboratory

experiments, the authors examined a proposed effect of shared participation in novel and arousing activities on experienced relationship quality. The questionnaire and

survey studies found predicted correlations of reported shared "exciting" activities

and relationship satisfaction plus their predicted mediation by relationship

boredom. In all 3 experiments, the authors found predicted greater increases in

experienced relationship quality from before to after participating together in a 7-

min novel and arousing (vs. a more mundane) task. Comparison with a no-activity

control showed the effect was due to the novel-arousing task. The same effect was

found on ratings of videotaped discussions before and after the experimental task.

Finally, all results remained after controlling for relationship social desirability.

Results bear on general issues of boredom and excitement in relationships and the

role of such processes in understanding the typical early decline of relationship

quality after the honeymoon period.

Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., Impett, E. A., & Asher, E. R. (2004). What do you do when things go right? The intrapersonal and interpersonal benefits of sharing positive events.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 228-245.

Four studies examined the intrapersonal and interpersonal consequences of seeking out others when good things happen (i.e., capitalization). Two studies showed that

communicating personal positive events with others was associated with increased daily positive affect and well-being, above and beyond the impact of the positive

event itself and other daily events. Moreover, when others were perceived to respond actively and constructively (and not passively or destructively) to capitalization

attempts, the benefits were further enhanced. Two studies found that close

relationships in which one’s partner typically responds to capitalization attempts

enthusiastically were associated with higher relationship well-being (e.g., intimacy, daily marital satisfaction). The results are discussed in terms of the theoretical and empirical importance of understanding how people “cope” with positive events,

cultivate positive emotions, and enhance social bonds.

Funk, J. L. & Rogge, R. D. (2007). Testing the ruler with item response theory: Increasing precision of measurement for relationship satisfaction with the Couples Satisfaction Index. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 572-583.

The present study took a critical look at a central construct in couples research:

relationship satisfaction. Eight well-validated self-report measures of relationship

satisfaction, including the Marital Adjustment Test (MAT; H. J. Locke & K. M.

Wallace, 1959), the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS; G. B. Spanier, 1976), and an

additional 75 potential satisfaction items, were given to 5,315 online participants.

Using item response theory, the authors demonstrated that the MAT and DAS

provided relatively poor levels of precision in assessing satisfaction, particularly

given the length of those scales. Principal-components analysis and item response

theory applied to the larger item pool were used to develop the Couples Satisfaction Index (CSI) scales. Compared with the MAS and the DAS, the CSI scales were

shown to have higher precision of measurement (less noise) and correspondingly

greater power for detecting differences in levels of satisfaction. The CSI scales

demonstrated strong convergent validity with other measures of satisfaction and

excellent construct validity with anchor scales from the nomological net surrounding satisfaction, suggesting that they assess the same theoretical construct as do prior

scales. Implications for research are discussed.

Scale:

Low High

1 2 3 4 5 1. How well does your partner meet your

needs?

1 2 3 4 5

2. In general, how satisfied are you with

your relationship?

3. How good is your relationship

1 2 3 4 5 compared to most?

1 2 3 4 5 4. How often do you wish you hadn’t

gotten into this relationship?

5. To what extent has your relationship

1 2 3 4 5 met your original expectations?

6. How much do you love your partner? 1 2 3 4 5

1 2 3 4 5 7. How many problems are there in your

relationship?

Scoring:

Items 4 and 7 are reverse-scored.

Scoring is kept continuous. The higher the score, the more satisfied the respondent is with his/her relationship.

表单设计实验五

表单实验五 一、实验题目: 表单创建 二、实验目的与要求: (1)掌握类、对象的设计及调用方法等。 (2)掌握用表单向导设计单表、多表表单的操作。 (3)掌握用表单设计器设计表单的方法。 (4)掌握重要表单控件的使用和使用控件生成器生成控件。 三、实验内容: 实验5-1设计一个用户登录表单,在表单上创建一个组合框和一个文本框,从组合框选择用 户名,在文本框中输入口令,三次不正确退出。 方法步骤: 图7.1 (1)新建表单Form1,从表单控件工具栏中拖入两个标签Label1、Label2,两个命令按钮Command1、Command2,以及一个组合框控件Combo1和一个文本框控件Text1。并按图7.1调整好其位置和大小。 (2)设置Label1的Caption属性值为“用户名”,Label2的Caption属性值为“密码”,Command1、Command2的Caption属性值分别为“登录”和“退出”。Form1的Caption属性值为“登录”。 (3)设置Combo1的RowSourceType属性为“1-值”,RowSource属性为“孙瑞,刘燕”,Text1的PasswordChar属性为“*”。 (4)在Form1的Init Event过程中加入如下代码: public num num=0 在Command1的Click Event过程中加入如下的程序代码: if (alltrim(https://www.360docs.net/doc/e93312211.html,bo1.value)=="孙瑞" and alltrim(thisform.text1.value)=="123456") or (alltrim(https://www.360docs.net/doc/e93312211.html,bo1.value)=="刘燕" and alltrim(thisform.text1.value)=="abcdef") thisform.release do 主菜单.mpr else

企业大数据表单的向导式UI设计

企业大数据表单的向导式UI设计 Ray Liu 2013-02-20 前言 (2) 第一章向导式UI (3) 本节总结 (5) 第二章改进型的向导式UI (6) 本节总结 (7) 第三章向导式UI的缺点 (7) 结束语 (7)

前言 企业内部的信息管理系统,由于业务的复杂性,导致我们的一张订单中往往需要填写大量的数据信息。先来看一下excel2007中的模版中的DHL EMailShip订单 上面仅仅是一个tab中的内容,需要完整填的话,还有invoice, packingList等等,作为一个新手,填写这么多的数据可真是让人头大的事情啊。

第一章向导式UI 对于新手来说,做上述复杂单据无疑是个漫长的学习和适应的过程,由此,我想到了是否可以参考现今电商网站的购物页面,采用创建向导的形式来创建订单,目的有3点: 1.新手可以快速上手 2.流程固化,不易出错 3.数据的分块填写,减少注意力分散 举例:填写一张销售订单(excel2007中的Sales Order模版) 传统的非向导式的UI如下,用户直接在一个form中填写完所有信息。

向导式的UI如下: 第一步 第二步 第三步 第四步 点击提交,我们就创建了一张完整的销售订单了,效果如图1 一样

本节总结 对于新手,向导式UI无疑是好的。再次重申其目的 1.新手可以快速上手 2.流程固化,不易出错 3.数据的分块填写,减少注意力分散 OK,对于这个例子,你也许会疑问,我直接填数据也很直观啊,我不觉得这么麻烦的跳转UI填 来填去的就是方便了。 对,非常对,假设你入门了,精通了,变老手了,你愿意每次都这样一项一项的点击去填数据么?我不愿意,非常不愿意。 So,我们需要改进型(更友好)的向导式UI。

EasyFlow3.7.1版 表单向导使用手册

鼎捷系统集团控股有限公司 3.7.1版表单向导使用手册 文件编号: 文件版次: 1.1.1.0 文件日期:2014年8月28日

文件制/修订履历 版次日期说明作者备注1.1.1.02014.08.28第一版

目录 一、新表单设计区-建立新表单 (2) 二、表单向导控制组件说明 (17) 1、Label (17) 2、Textbox (17) 3、Dropdown下拉选单控件 (21) 4、TEXTAREA (26) 5、Radio Button控件 (26) 6、Checkbox控件 (27) 7、Datetime日期控件 (30) 8、部门及员工控件 (34) 9、OpenQuery开窗控件 (36) 10、Button控制组件 (40) 11、Grid单身控件 (41) 12、图片控件 (44) 13、PASSWORD密码控制组件 (45) 14、Line线条 (45) 15、隐藏字段控件 (47) 二、表单重新设计区 (48) 三、表单复制区 (51) 四、修改自定义表单主旨区 (58) 五、表单名称修改功能 (60) 六、表单向导删除功能 (62) 七、新增删除历程查询 (64)

当您欲使用3.7.1版的电子表单设计向导,可以点选在电子表单设计工具下的电子表单设计向导后,会进入以下画面: 目前共有七个功能: 「新表单设计区」、「表单重新设计区」、「表单复制区」及「修改自定义表单主旨区」、「修改表单名称区」、「表单删除区」及「表单删除历程」 以下章节将逐一介绍这七大功能:

一、新表单设计区-建立新表单

1、Step1:输入「表单代号」、「表单简称」、「表单全称」,选择「表单类别」。 表单代号命名注意事项: 作业代号的命名方式:[3码开发代号]+[3码公司代号][2码程序流水号]其中[3码开发代号]:名称,ex.ODM [3码公司代号]:名称,ex:IBM 则表单代号命名为:ODMIBM01

习题5 项目管理器、设计器和向导的使用

习题5 项目管理器、设计器和向导的使用 6.要把在项目管理器之外创建的文件包含在项目文件中,需要使用项目管理器的 8.下列关于“事件”的叙述中,错误的是_________。 A. Visual FoxPro中基类的事件可以由用户创建 B. Visual FoxPro中基类的事件是有系统预先定义好的,不可由用户创建 C.事件是一种事先定义好的特定动作,由用户或系统激活 D.鼠标的单击、双击、移动和键盘上按键的按下均可激活某个时间

习题5 项目管理器、设计器和向导的使用- 133 - 11.若某表单中有一个文本框Text1和一个命令按钮CommandGroup1,其中,命令按钮 组包含了Command1和Command2两个命令按钮。如果要在命令按钮Command1的某个方法中访问文本框Text1的V alue属性值,下列式子中正确的是_________。 12.在表单中加入两个命令按钮Command1和Command2;编写Command1的Click事 件代码如下,则当单击Command1后_________。 https://www.360docs.net/doc/e93312211.html,mand2.Enabled = .F. A. Command1命令按钮不能激活 B. Command2命令按钮不能激活 C.事件代码无法执行 D.命令按钮组中的第2个命令按钮不能激活 13.V isual FoxPro提供了3种方式来创建表单,它们分别是表单向导创建表单;使用 _________创建一个新的表单或修改一个已经存在的表单;使用“表单”菜单中的快速表单命令创建一个简单的表单。 17.在运行某个表单时,下列有关表单事件引发次序的叙述中正确的是_________。 A.先Activate事件,然后Init事件,最后Load事件 B.先Activate事件,然后Load事件,最后Init事件 C.先Init事件,然后Activate事件,最后Load事件 D.先Load事件,然后Init事件,最后Activate事件 18.在表单中添加了某些控件后,除了通过属性窗口为其设置各种控件外,也可以通过 19.在当前目录下有M.PRG和M.SCX两个文件,在执行命令DO M后,实际运行的

c语言 创建、运行和修改表单

实验(六)创建、运行和修改表单 电科081班级张辉 NO.:8 实验目的: 1.掌握利用向导创建表单的方法。 2.掌握为对象设置属性和编写事件代码的技能。 3.通过运行由VFP向导生成的表单了解数据管理的功能。 实验要求: 1.使用一对多表单向导,以“订单”表为父表,“订单明细”表为子表生成订单表单。 2.将表单的“订单号:”标签设置为红色。 3.右击表单能弹出一个信息框。 4.运行订“订单”表单,通过操作了解订单向导的这一实例提供的数据管理功能:浏览记录、查找记录、编辑记录、打印报表、添加记录和删除记录。 实验准备: 1.阅读主教材6.1.2节和6.3节。 2.创建好“订货”数据库(见实验3-2) 实验步骤: 6-1 创建表单:选定菜单命令“工具/向导/表单”,即显示“向导选取”对话框→在列表中选定“一对多表单向导”选项,即出现“一对多表单向导”对话框→以“订货”数据库的“订单表”为父表并选用全部字段(图 a)→以“订单明细”表为子表并选用货号和数量字段→单击“完成”按钮(图 b),然后将表单文件取名为“订单”(图 c)。保存后表单设计器如图2.6.1所示→参照图2.6.2缩小表格,移动对象。

6-2 标签设置红色:单击“订单号:”标签,随之属性窗口的对象组合框中即显示“LBL订单号1”→在属性列表中选定ForeColor,并在属性设置框中输入255,0,0.

6-3 为Form1的RightClick事件编写代码:双击表单窗口打开代码编辑窗口,在对象组合框中即显示Form1选项,在过程组合框中选定RightClick事件,然后在列表框中输入以下代码。 6-4 运行表单:在常用工具栏中单击“运行”按钮即显示如下表单(图 2.6.2)。右击表单会弹出一个信息窗口如下所示:

phpcmsv9不用插件打造留言板,而是用表单向导模块和dialog

不用插件打造意见反馈(留言板),先给个图: 表单向导+dialog 一、表单向导 1.登陆Phpcmsv9后台https://www.360docs.net/doc/e93312211.html,/index.php?m=admin 2.模块》模块管理》表单向导》添加表单向导

1)名称::意见反馈(请输入表单向导名称) 2)表名:message(请填写表名) 3)简介:(这个可以不填) 4)下三个可以不用改 5)允许游客提交表单:要选是 7)模板选择:

这个你一定要提前做好模板, 比如我的是show_box.html, 这里要注意模板命名要以show_开头 8)js调用使用的模板:这里不做介绍,可以不理它了。 3,下面,确定。如果图 功能如下: 1)信息列表:用来查看留言信息,现在不用 2)添加字段:主要用这个,我们要添加三个字段 分别是留言标题(title),联系邮箱(email),留言内容(content) 添加:字段 ---字段类型: ----字段类型 ----字段别名 ----数据校验正则(这个的话看你自己的需求来用) 其他的可以不写 最后》提交

三、模板 找到phpcms\templates\default\formguide 新建模板show_box.html

{loop $forminfos_data $field $info} {if $info['formtype']=='omnipotent'} {loop $forminfos_data $_fm $_fm_value} {if $_fm_value['iscomnipotent']} {php $info['form'] = str_replace('{'.$_fm.'}',$_fm_value['form'],$info['form']);} {/if} {/loop} {/if} {/loop}

数据表单试题---答案

数据表单试题一、单选题 1.在VFP中,表单(Form)是指(D)。 A)数据库中各个表的清单 B)一个表中各个记录的清单 C)数据库查询的列表 D)窗口界面 2.表单文件的扩展名为(C)。 A).DBC B).DBF C).SCX D).PJX 3.下列关于“表单”说法中正确的是(B )。 A)任何表单都隶属于一个项目 B)有的表单可游离于任何项目而独立存在 C)任何表单都含数据表的字段 D)表单不属于容器控件 4.下列关于“表单”说法中错误的是(A )。 A)表单只能含1个数据表中的字段 B)表单可含2个数据表中的字段 C)表单可含3个数据表中的字段 D)表单可不含数据表中的字段

5.下列关于“快速表单”说法中正确的(A )。 A)“快速表单”只适用于1个数据表的情形,即字段不能来自不同的数据表。 B)“快速表单”能设计2个数据表中的字段 C)“快速表单”能设计3个数据表中的字段 D)“快速表单”只适用于不含数据表中的字段的情形 6.在表单上同时选中多个控件的方法是(D )。 A)用鼠标依次单击各个控件 B)先单击第一个控件,左手按住Alt键并保持,用鼠标依次单击其余各控件 C)先单击第一个控件,左手按住Ctrl键并保持,用鼠标依次单击其余各控件 D)先单击第一个控件,左手按住Shift键并保持,用鼠标依次单击其余各控件 7.复选框有(B )种状态。 A)1 B)2 C)3 D)4 [page] 8.有两种形式的组合框,即下拉组合框和下拉列表框,下列说法错误的是(D )。

A)当Style的值为“0”时为下拉组合框;当Style的值为“2”时为下拉列表框 B)在下拉组合框中,用户既可以从中选择一个选项,也可直接在文本框中输入一个新值 C)在下拉列表框中,只允许用户从它的下拉列表中选择一个选项,而不允许用户在文本框中输入新值 D)在下拉列表框中,既允许用户从它的下拉列表中选择一个选项,也允许用户在文本框中输入新值 9.下列关于微调控件说法中不正确的是(C )。 A)可以使用微调控件和文本框来微调数值 B)可以使用微调控件和文本框来微调日期 C)微调控件属于容器类控件 D)增减的步长取决于属性Increment的值 10.如果要为控件设置焦点,则下列属性值是真(.T.)的是(B )。 A)Enabled和Default B)Enabled和Visible C)Default和Cancel D)Visible和Default 11.下列控件中,不能设置数据源的是()。 A)复选框 B)列表框

表单设计器功能设计

表单自定义设计器 1设计思路 1.1表单自定义功能的误区 1、关于成本:表单自定义一般容易实现的仅布局、字段的增减、简单的脚本控制等,但有很多诸如复杂脚本控制、自动计算、特殊逻辑验证、主从关系,复杂基础数据选择(过滤、合并)、与其它功能模块的交互等等需求,自定义工具都不能很轻易地解决,最终可能带来的代价是重做,甚至推翻整个系统架构重新实现,付出成本是预计成本的2-4倍以上均有可能。建议采用对此类复杂需求通过关联创建人定义的SQL语句来实现。 2、表单自定义功能实现的方式一般是数据库表中预制了很多字段或者是一个表中的记录存储为ID、字段名、值、字段类型,而且值的类型往往是字符型,这些做法给数据的查询统计及SQL优化带来的是非常大的性能损失和阻力,业务系统数据量不大的时候看不出,一旦数据业务表大到一定程度的时候,性能瓶颈就会出现。我们知道需要工作流的业务系统都是大量用户和大规模业务数据的。对于表单自定义做法,性能瓶颈是一定要考虑的; 3、表单自定义往往实现的是一个数据实体的增、删、改,但对于一个系统来讲一个表单仅仅是一个功能点而已,这个功能点对于整个系统来讲远不是那么单纯的,有可能一个数据实体的资料分别在多个表单里进行更新和维护,自定义逻辑往往是处理不了它们之间的冲突,还有查询和统计分析,这些是需要关联很多基础数据、关联其它业务数据。自定义表单功能本身也只是从功能特性的角度去出发,对于系统复杂的实体关系、业务模式、设计模式的支持几乎为零,一个高质量系统需要的因素基本实现不了; 4、企业使用表单自定义工具的时候往往已经有了很多的系统,比如HR、CRM甚至ERP系统,很多关联数据会是来自于这些系统的数据。表单自定义工

相关主题
相关文档
最新文档
{$info['name']}{if $info['star']} *{/if} {$info['form']} {$info['tips']}